Samuel Noh, William R. Avison: другие произведения.

Asian Immigrants and the Stress Process: A Study of Koreans in Canada

Сервер "Заграница": [Регистрация] [Найти] [Рейтинги] [Обсуждения] [Новинки] [Помощь]
  • Комментарии: 2, последний от 08/08/2014.
  • © Copyright Samuel Noh, William R. Avison (kazgugnk@yahoo.com)
  • Обновлено: 30/01/2011. 150k. Статистика.
  • Статья: Канада
  • Иллюстрации: 1 штук.
  •  Ваша оценка:


      
       Asian Immigrants and the Stress Process: A Study of Koreans in Canada*
       SAMUEL NOH WILLIAM R. AVISON
       The University of Western Ontario
       Journal of Health and Social Behavior 1996, Vol. 37 (June): 192-206
       The stress process paradigm is one of the most widely held perspectives in the sociology of mental health. While this model has been applied to a large number of research problems, relatively little work has examined whether the paradigm can assist in understanding the link between socially induced stressors and their consequences for the mental health of immigrants. In this paper, we employ a stress process formulation to examine the interplay among stressors, psychological and social resources, and psychological distress among a large sample of Korean immigrants living in Toronto, Canada. Data from a longitudinal study of over 600 respondents indicate that the stress process paradigm provides a useful perspective for understanding how chronic stressors associated with the immigration experience manifest themselves in psychological distress. Our results suggest that social and psychological resources have important deterrent effects on the experience of stressors and their subsequent distressful consequences. Ethnic social support and mastery are especially important factors in this process.
      
       Over the past few decades, large influxes of immigrants from Asian countries have reshaped the nature of ethnic dynamics in North American societies (Dumas 1985; Hurh and Kim 1988, 1990; Portes and Rumbaut 1990). In Canada, Asian immigrants ac­counted for less than 10 percent of the total number of immigrants until the 1960s; however, by 1973, the proportion of Asian migrants had increased to more than 20 percent of the total immigration. In the 1980s, immigrants from Asian countries accounted for approximately 50 percent of the total
       * An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1994 Society for the Study of Social Problems Annual Meeting in Los Angeles, CA. This study was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (Award 410-87-0821) to Samuel Noh and William R. Avison and an Ontario Mental Health Foundation Senior Research Fellowship to William R. Avison. Correspondence should be addressed to Samuel Noh, Room 2535 Elborn College, Applied Health Sciences, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6G 1H1; e-mail: snoh@biostats.uwo.ca.
       immigration to Canada and more than 10 percent of the national growth (Dumas 1985). There are at least two reasons for studying the mental health of Asian immigrants. First, there are relatively few large-scale, system­atic studies of these immigrant populations. In the United States, studies of minority mental health have been largely limited to African American and Hispanic groups (Vega and Rumbaut 1991), although work by Kuo (1976), Kuo and Tsai (1986), and Sue and Morishima (1992) are notable exceptions. In Canada, minority mental health is only now attracting wider attention (cf. Beiser and Wood 1986). Second, studying the mental health of Asian immigrants provides an opportunity to assess whether the stress process is a theoretical framework that can be used to understand the link between socially induced stressors and their consequences for the mental health of immigrants. Much of the current literature that has supported the stress process formulation has been limited to non-Hispanic, White communities. Few at­tempts have been made to examine cultural variations in the effects of social stressors on mental health.
      
       192
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       193
      
      
      
       In this paper, we present results that assist us in better understanding the process through which stress manifests itself among Asian immigrants. Specifically, we explore how life stressors and coping resources interact with one another to influence the psychological well-being of a large sample of Korean immigrants in Toronto, Canada.
       MENTAL HEALTH AMONG KOREAN IMMIGRANTS
       At present, our understanding of the mental health implications of international migration is severely limited; moreover, information on Asian immigrants is even more scant (Sue and Morishima 1982; Vega and Rumbaut 1991). Earlier studies of "treated cases" almost invariably report findings that are contrary to the belief that Asian immigrants suffered from major mental health problems (Berk and Hirata 1973; Jew and Brody 1967; Kintano 1969; Sue and McKinney 1975). However, the reduced rate of admissions to hospitals for psychiatric treatment seems more likely to be a reflection of Asians' cultural tendency toward underutilization of mental health services as well as a fear of the stigma associated with psychiatric disorders in most Asian societies, rather than an indicator of greater well-being among Asians (Lin et al. 1982; Sue and Morishima 1982; Vega and Rumbaut 1991). Thus, in addition to studies of treated cases, there appears to be a need for studies of Asian immigrants that employ sampling strategies which are more likely to generate representative samples from the community.
       There are, however, relatively fewer epide-miologic investigations of Asian immigrants resident in the community. To our knowl­edge, only three epidemiologic studies have been carried out to examine mental health problems of Korean immigrants in the United States and Canada. The first epidemiologic study of Korean immigrants was conducted by Kuo (Kuo 1984; Kuo and Tsai 1986). Samples of Asian Americans--Chinese, Fili­pinos, Japanese, and Koreans--were selected in Seattle and were administered the Center for Epidemiologic Studies of Depression (CES-D) scale to estimate the prevalence of depression in these four ethnic communities. Its overall prevalence was slightly higher compared to the prevalence reported for
       non-Hispanic Whites, but somewhat lower than those for African Americans and His­panic Americans (cf. Eaton and Kessler 1981; Frerichs, Aneshensel, and Clark 1981; Gary et al. 1989). Kuo (1984) found that the prevalence of depression in Korean immi­grants in his Seattle sample was about twice the rate in each of the other three Asian groups. However, the findings of this study are difficult to generalize because the study involved only 110 subjects in the Korean community in Seattle. A more comprehensive study of Korean immigrants' mental health was conducted by Hurh and Kim (1988, 1990). Although they report that the preva­lence of depression among Korean immi­grants in Chicago was somewhat lower than the rate from Kuo's study, it was nevertheless comparable to the Seattle study.
       Recently, in a study of 860 Koreans in Toronto, Noh and his colleagues (Noh, Speechley, et al. 1992; Noh, Wu, et al. 1992) reported that the prevalence of depression was between 3.1 percent and 5.9 percent, figures that are similar to the rates of depression reported in the Stirling County Study in Canada (Murphy, Sobol, and Neff 1984), the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) stud­ies in the United States (Regier, Myers, and Kramer 1984; Robins and Regier 1991), and in a large epidemiologic survey in Edmonton, Canada (Bland, Newman, and Orn 1988). These studies reported estimates of preva­lence ranging from 5.1 percent to 5.6 percent in Canada and 3.5 percent to 5.6 percent in the United States.
       THE STRESS PROCESS AND THE MENTAL HEALTH OF KOREAN IMMIGRANTS
       Recent studies of social stress focus on the ways in which social stressors from various sources result in ill health of various forms. A basic conceptual framework of the stress process has been proposed by Pearlin and his associates (1981). They illustrate the roles of and interrelationships among three major constructs: stressors, mediators or coping resources, and distress or health outcome. Briefly, the model portrays the ways in which the harmful consequences of social stressors vary substantially across individuals and social groups, and explains how such varia­tions are determined by psychological and
      
       194
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       social coping resources, including personal­ity, coping efforts and skills, and social supports. Studies in many disciplines provide empirical evidence supporting the stress-mediating and stress-buffering roles of coping resources.
       More recently, the research in social stress has addressed the question of how coping resources function to mediate or buffer the potentially harmful impact of social stressors (Wheaton 1985). Empirical studies, however, have presented mixed, if not contradictory, findings and conclusions regarding the stress-buffering role of social support (Aneshensel and Stone 1982; Gore 1981; House 1981; Lin et al. 1979; Thoits 1982, 1986; Turner 1981).
       Several competing hypotheses or models have been proposed to explicate the ways in which social and psychological resources reduce the adverse impacts of life stressors (Dohrenwend and Dohrenwend 1981; Ensel and Lin 1991; Lin and Ensel 1989; Wheaton 1985). Although these models vary to some degree, Ensel and Lin (1991) have synthe­sized them into two global models. The first is a deterring model in which preexisting coping resources may act to reduce the impact of stressors on distress in three distinct ways. Resources may directly reduce distress but do so independently of stressors. Second, re­sources may result in a suppression of stressors that then results in reduced distress. Third, stress conditioning may occur in which variations in levels of resources in conjunc­tion with subsequent stressors condition the effect of stressors on distress. These deterring models are consistent with Paykel's (1978: 251) remark that it is not just the presence of life stressors that is significant for mental distress, but that "the soil in which it falls" conditions a stressor's potential conse­quences. In this view, then, preexisting resources play a crucial role in the stress process.
       The other global model is the coping model in which the relationship between stressors and resources is reversed. This conception is consistent with the view that stressors "... do not necessarily impact upon people directly but may, instead, exert their effects through a wider context of life strains" (Pearlin et al. 1981:339), where stressors or strains represent life situations in which important coping resources are severely jeopardized or compromised. Again, there are three distinct versions of this model. First, the
       harmful effects of stressors may be counter­acted by coping resources that are triggered by the experience of stress. In this model, increases in stress produce increases in coping resources that lead to a subsequent reduction in distressful outcomes. Alternatively, an increase in stressors may be associated with a deterioration of coping resources, where stressors are predicted to have a negative effect on resources; that is, coping resources mediate the effects of stressors on distress. Third, the stress-buffering effects of resources emerge when there is an interaction between prior stressors and current resources. Clearly, the viability of any of these models requires verification and, to date, empirical data have supported two of the coping (deterioration and stress-buffering) models and one of the deterring model (the independent model) (cf. Lin, Dean, and Ensel 1986; Ensel and Lin 1991). Whether these findings generalize to immigrant populations is a question that we address in this paper.
       Applying the Paradigm to Immigrants' Mental Health
       Moritsugu and Sue (1983) and Vega and Rumbaut (1991) have proposed a basic framework for studying minority mental health that is based on the stress process perspective. Social stress research assumes that life stressors do not occur randomly; rather, difficulties in life are systematically and disproportionately distributed across indi­viduals and sociologically definable groups. From this perspective, minority status or marginality itself can be considered a signifi­cant stressor (Moritsugu and Sue 1983; Vega and Rumbaut 1991). One prediction that emerges from this formulation is that immi­grants will experience more mental health complications than native-born individuals "... because the uprooting process itself produces an excessive amount of social stress" (Kuo and Tsai 1986:134).
       Although several studies provide confirma­tion of the adverse psychological ramifica­tions of the migratory and uprooting process, Kuo and Tsai (1986) contend that there is empirical evidence suggesting the contrary (e.g., Fitinger and Schwartz 1981; Cochrane and Stopes-Roe 1977; Burnam et al. 1987). Indeed, some scientists believe that immigra­tion policies in the United States and Canada tend to select healthy Asian immigrants who
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       195
      
      
      
       can be characterized as having a "hardy personality" (Burnam et al. 1987; Kuo and Tsai 1986). Furthermore, immigrants can and do institute their own social networks and social institutions and in so doing establish substantial ethnic social support networks that allow them to live relatively separated from the larger society (Kuo and Tsai 1986). In fact, those immigrants who maintain their own ethnic culture and networks while also adopting the cultural and social networks of the host society tend to exhibit better health, compared to either "assimilationists" or "traditionalists" (Hurh and Kim 1984, 1988; Rumbaut 1991; Vega and Rumbaut 1991). While these explanations seem viable, their validity has yet to be established in empirical investigations.
       However, before assessing the utility of these explanations, it seems necessary to modify the stress formulation to consider life situations and experiences that are particularly relevant to immigrants. In particular, we contend that the application of a stress process formulation requires a careful specification of the particular social stressors to which immigrants are exposed and the kinds of social and psychological resources that are available to them.
       Social stressors. In specifying stressors in a study of immigrants, it is important to consider both acute stressors and chronic strains as po­tential sources of life stress. As well, it seems clear that the kinds of chronic strains that may be most relevant to the experiences of new im­migrants are likely to differ from the kinds of role strains that are usually measured in most studies of the stress process. While immigrants experience stress brought on by discrete life events and hassles, stress can also be induced by chronic, ongoing difficulties. New immi­grants are prone to adjustment problems such as cultural conflicts, social integration and as­similation, role changes and identity crises, low socioeconomic status, and the experience of ra­cial discrimination (Fitinger and Schwartz 1981; Kuo 1976; Hull 1979; Sue and Morishima 1982). In this study, we take into account strains as­sociated with adaptation in the following areas of life: acquisition of new language; sense of isolation; homesickness or nostalgia; sense of marginality; experienced social discrimination; financial or economic hardships; changes in role structure and role identity; and problems in the family due to cultural changes.
       Social resources. A substantial body of
       research has examined the effects that various social resources have on individuals' mental health. Several reviews of the literature on social support and social resources have provided incontrovertible evidence on the important role played by social support in the stress process (e.g., House, Landis, and Umberson 1988; Lin et al. 1986; Sarason, Pierce, and Sarason 1994).
       When studying recent immigrants, the role of social support cannot be assessed in a unidimensional manner. By definition, the uprooting process involves disruption of immigrants' social interaction and fractures in social networks; however, some argue that the consequences of such uprooting may not be as serious as previously thought (Kuo and Tsai 1986). For example, many recent Asian immigrants have "chain-migrated" to rejoin their families and relatives in the new country. In addition, new immigrants may also actively participate in social interaction to establish new social networks following migration. These newly established networks may be confined to immigrants' own ethnic communities or they may extend beyond ethnic boundaries to the larger communities of the host society as immigrants proceed further in terms of structural assimilation (Gordon 1962). Given these possibilities, an interesting question is whether the ethnic origin of social supports influences the stress-mediating or buffering functions of resources. There is both theory and research that is relevant to this possibility. Lin (1992) has developed a theory of social resources which suggests that the degree of homophily (shared social and cultural characteristics) among members of a social group may have important implications for the provision of social resources and social support. He argues that further research on this possibility is warranted. A review by Kuo and Tsai (1986) provides conflicting evidence. While there is support for the position that intense involve­ments and ties confined within ethnic net­works can be dysfunctional (Granovetter 1973; Hirsch 1980; Quizumbing 1982; Well-man 1981; Wilcox 1981), other researchers present evidence for the positive functions of ethnic networks and supports (Banchevska 1981; Koranyi 1981; Kuo and Tsai 1986; Murphy 1973; Portes 1983). Accordingly, it is important to specify these two sources of social resources in any study of the stress process among immigrants and to assess their
      
       196
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       relative impact on health outcomes. To our knowledge, our study is the first to specify ethnic and nonethnic sources of social support and to compare their efficacy. Our cross-sectional analyses suggest that ethnic support is very important for the mental health of Korean immigrants, while the degree of nonethnic social support is not associated with the health outcome (Noh, Wu, and Avison 1994).
       Psychological resources. For some time, researchers have been interested in the roles that psychological resources play in the stress process. In this context, constructs that are associated with personal agency have been a central theoretical and empirical focus. These constructs include locus of control (Lefcourt 1981; Rotter 1966), mas­tery (Pearlin and Schooler 1978), and helplessness (Seligman 1975). In addition, resources that relate to self-esteem have also occupied a central position in the stress process formulation. The theoretical impor­tance of this construct has been demon­strated convincingly by Rosenberg (1979) and Kaplan (1975).
       Social stress research provides a substan­tial empirical basis for the belief that personal resources such as mastery or self-esteem are associated with psychological distress among persons experiencing chronic life strains (Avison 1995; Pearlin et al. 1981; Turner and Roszell 1994; Turner and Noh 1983). Mastery refers to the degree to which a person views life as being under his or her own control as opposed to being fatalistically determined. The concept thus describes an intrapsychic resource that influ­ences, and is influenced by, one's percep­tions about past and current experiences in controlling and managing life's challenges. To this extent, the concept shares much with that of locus of control and learned helpless­ness. These concepts are hypothesized to be strongly connected to depression and other forms of psychological distress (Noh and Turner 1987; Seligman 1975; Turner and Avison 1992b).
       The salience of these constructs for the mental health of minority group members has been discussed in an excellent review by Moritsugu and Sue (1983). Because the uprooting process involves changes in social and cultural climates, new immigrants may be frustrated by the fact that their social skills and defense mechanisms are not typically
       functional or socially acceptable in their new setting (Moritsugu and Sue 1983; Sue 1977; Sue and Zane 1980). When such frustrating experiences are repeated over time, self-esteem and mastery can be eroded, thus increasing vulnerability to social stressors.
       Despite the conceptual implications of psychological resources, their role in in­fluencing psychological well-being in immigrants and minority groups has not been explicated extensively (Moritsugu and Sue 1983). Kuo and Tsai (1986) found that sense of control was directly associated with both stressors and depression. In addition, sense of control also had a significant moderating effect on the association between stressors and depression. It appears, then, that there is a need for a careful examination of the roles of these resources among immigrants.
       THE KOREAN MENTAL HEALTH STUDY
       Research Design
       The data for this study are drawn from the Korean Mental Health Study (KMHS), a two-wave panel study of life strains and mental health problems among Korean immi­grants in Toronto. It has been estimated that about 35,000 Korean immigrants are now residing in Canada, with a majority (about 20,000) living in metropolitan Toronto (Shin 1991). Data were obtained through interviews conducted in the summers of 1990 and 1991. All measures were translated into Korean and then independently translated back to English to ensure their validity in Korean.
       The Directory of the Korean Society of Toronto, listing over 4,000 households in the metropolitan area of Toronto, was the pri­mary source for the sampling frame. A total of 1,039 households were selected using a simple random sampling method. Within each selected family, only one adult (aged 18 or older) was randomly selected. Those who migrated before the age of 16 were excluded because their migratory adjustment experi­ences were likely to be substantially different from those of older adult immigrants. Inter­views were conducted by trained bilingual interviewers following structured interview schedules.
       In the first wave of the study (Wl), a
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       197
      
      
      
       sample of 860 subjects provided relatively complete sets of information, representing a participation rate of 86 percent. Males (53%) and females (47%) were almost equally represented in the sample. A majority of the sample (55.3%) was 36 to 55 years old while 19.2 percent were older than 55. These figures closely parallel the sex and age distributions found in the 1986 Canadian Census, except that the elderly were some­what underrepresented in the sample. Most respondents were either currently married (82.6%) or never married (10.2%). More than half (52.6%) of the sample had some postsecondary education. This figure was higher for males (59.9%) than for females (44.2%). Most men (80.5%) and women (67.6%) reported that they either worked outside the home or had their own business. These percentages are roughly comparable to data reported by Shin (1991) for all adult Korean immigrants in Canada.
       The majority of our sample (68.7%) had immigrated to Canada between the ages of 26 and 35. Of course, age of migration is constrained somewhat by our sampling crite­rion because we excluded those Koreans who had immigrated before age 16. On average, respondents had been living in Canada for 12 years. Despite this, only a few (19%) had received formal education after immigrating to Canada.
       A total of 609 respondents (71% of the baseline sample) participated in second wave interviews (W2); 116 (13.5%) refused to be reinterviewed, 133 (15.5%) were lost to follow-up (mostly through geographic moves out of the catchment area), and 2 (.2%) had died. As with most community surveys, there were trends indicating greater loss rates among younger and unemployed subjects as well as those with shorter length of residency in Canada. However, these differences were not found with those who were located but refused to be interviewed. Indeed, the refus­ers closely resembled the participants on all demographic and stress process variables that are used in this study.
       To explore further the impact of attrition, we regressed psychological distress (at Wl) on demographic variables, stressors, and resource variables. Interaction terms were computed between each of the stressors and resource variables and a dummy variable that identified the participants and nonpartici-pants. Statistically significant interaction
       terms would therefore reflect attrition bias. In no case, however, was there any evidence of a significant interaction term. Accordingly, we are confident that attrition from Wl to W2 did not introduce significant biases in our results.
       Measurement
       Psychological distress. The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) is a 20-item scale that assesses current level of depressive mood. A Korean version of the CES-D, translated by Hurh and Kim (1988), was adopted with minor modifi­cations. Elsewhere, we have reported detailed information on this scale's psychometric properties (Noh, Avison, and Kaspar 1992). However, as Lin (1989) and Noh, Avison, and Kaspar (1992) have noted, a potential source of bias has been identified on one dimension of the CES-D: Asians' responses to the positive affect items should be interpreted with caution because of the cultural tendency pf reporting unusually low levels of positive affect. Two methods have been suggested to eliminate this response bias: discarding the four positive items--an approach we adopted in the first wave of the KMHS (Noh, Speechley, et al. 1992; Noh, Wu, et al. 1992)--or rephrasing the positive items with negative content. In this study, Wl depression (CESD^ was measured with the 16-item scale (deleting the four positive items). The reliability of this scale was .89; after adjusting for the number of scale items, the mean score was 10.74 (s.d. = 9.00). In the second wave, the second method of correction was adopted to score CESD2. For example, the positive item "... hopeful about the future" was rephrased into a negative item, "... pessimistic about the future." The reliability of the revised 20-item CES-D was .92 and the mean was 10.60 (s.d. = 9.47). Thus, it appears that the two versions of the CES-D produce distributions with similar central tendencies and disper­sions.
       Stressors. We included assessments of both undesirable life events (LE) and chronic strains (CS). The number of life events during the 12-month period prior to Wl interviews (LE^ X = 1.93; s.d. = 1.97) was assessed using 22 items common to most checklists such as loss of a job, death of a family
      
       198
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       member, accident or injury, illness, a finan­cial problem, troubles with family members, and so forth. These events were drawn from the 31-item checklist used by Turner and Avison (1992a); the number of items was reduced to avoid duplication. In W2, respon­dents were asked to report on life events that occurred during the 12-month period between the two interviews (LE2; X = 1.53; s.d. = 1.90).
       A 31-item scale was devised to measure chronic strains associated with adaptation strains in seven major areas: language diffi­culty, homesickness, social isolation, social discrimination, sense of marginality, opportu­nities for occupational and financial mobility, and family problems. Subjects reported how often they felt stress resulting from each of the problems described. For this measure, the period of time over which the respondents experienced these difficulties was not speci­fied; these life difficulties could have per­sisted over any substantial length of time. Each response was coded on a 4-point scale ranging from "always or almost always" to "never." Scores for each item were equally weighted and summed to generate a compos­ite score of adaptational strain. Internal consistency reliabilities (alpha) of the seven subscales ranged from .77 to .89. The alpha reliability of the total strain measure (CS^ was .91. The mean of this measure was 65.96 (s.d. = 4.75). This measure was not repeated at W2.
       In developing this measure, we were concerned about the possible confounding of this construct with social support. Three observations have reassured us that this is not a serious problem. First, the zero-order correlations between chronic strain and our measures of social support are modest: -.15 with KSS! and -.26 with KSS2; -.20 with GSS! and -.19 with GSS2. Second, factor analyses reveal that our measure of strain forms a factor separate from social support. Finally, as we shall report, the effect of strain on depression persists, even with the inclusion of KSS in the regression analyses.
       Psychological resources. Mastery at Wl and W2 (MAX and MA2) was measured with a 7-item scale derived from Pearlin and Schooler (1978). The mean scores for МАХ and MA2 were 23.9 (s.d. = 5.7) and 24.1 (s.d. = 5.6), respectively. Self-esteem (SEX and SE2) was assessed with an abridged
       6-item version of Rosenberg's (1979) scale. These scales have been successfully used in a number of earlier large-scale studies (Noh and Turner 1987; Turner and Avison 1992a, 1992b; Turner and Noh 1983, 1988) and have consistently shown acceptable levels of reli­ability. In this study, the internal consistency reliabilities (Cronbach's alpha) of the mastery scale were .76 and .77 for Wl and W2, respectively. The reliability of the self-esteem scale was also acceptable (.79 for Wl and .82 for W2). The mean scores for SEi and SE2 were 23.2 (s.d. = 4.0) and 23.1 (s.d. = 4.3), respectively.
       Social resources. We measured perceived social support with the Provisions of Social Relations Scale (PSR) developed by Turner, Frankel, and Levin (1983). The content of this index is based on Weiss' (1974) concep­tual analysis of social relationships. For each of 15 items, respondents report the degree to which they perceive themselves to be socially supported by using a 5-point scale ranging from "very much like my experience" to "not at all like my experience."
       In order to distinguish between perceived social support from other Korean immigrants and support from the broader, nonethnic community, the PSR was evaluated by each respondent first with respect to their experi­ences of social relationships within Korean ethnic networks, and then again regarding their experiences in relation to family, friends, and со workers who were not of Korean origin. These two types of social support are hereafter referred to as Korean social support (KSS) and general social support (GSS). These variables were mea­sured at both Wl (KSS! and GSS^ and W2 (KSS2 and GSS2). The reliability (alpha) coefficients ranged from .84 to .88. Means for KSS were 3.7 (s.d. = 1.7) and 3.8 (s.d. = 1.7) for Wl and W2. For GSS, compara­ble values were 2.3 (s.d. = 1.7) and 2.7 (s.d. = 1.4) forWl and W2.
       Control variables that were incorporated into the analyses include age (interval mea­sure), sex, marital status (married vs. other), employment status (employed vs. other), education (interval measure), and the years of residency in Canada (interval measure). All control variables were measured at Wl. An appendix that includes the means and stan­dard deviations of all variables and a complete intercorrelation matrix is available on request.
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process 199
       RESULTS this analysis are presented as model 1 in

    Table 1. As can be seen, there is consider-

       Main Effects of Stressors and Resources able stability over one year in levels of
       Our analysis of these longitudinal data depressive symptoms among Korean immi-
       involved the computation of OLS regression grants in our sample. It is also the case that
       estimates of the effects of a series of none of the sociodemographic factors has a
       predictors on W2 depressive symptoms significant effect on changes in depression
       (CESD2). Our approach began with an scores.
       estimation of the stability of CES-D scores In the second step of this analysis, we
       over time by regressing CESD2 on CESDi, added Wl measures of chronic strain, life
       controlling for the array of sociodemo- events, mastery, self-esteem, ethnic social
       graphic factors listed above. The results of support, and general social support. As
       TABLE 1. Regression of CES-D at W2: Standardized Coefficients of Demographic Factors, Life Stress Measures, and Coping Resource Variables0
       0x08 graphic
    Predictors Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
       0x08 graphic
    Wave 1 Measures
       .455**
       .348**
       .272**

    (.481)

       (.367)
       (.287)

    -.019

       -.053
       -.041

    (-.355)

       (-.985)
       (-.772)

    -.101

       -.132*
       -.097*

    (-.072)

       (-.091)
       (-.067)

    -.005

       .002
       -.007

    (-.090)

       (.029)
       (-.116)

    -.077

       -.065
       -.050

    (-1.630)

       (-1.365)
       (-1.056)

    -.048

       -.087*
       -.081*

    (-.349)

       (-.500)
       (-.590)

    .002

       .055
       .067

    (.004)

       (.083)
       (.102)

    -.063

       -.040
       -.035

    (-.178)

       (-.115)
       (-.102)
      
       .091*
       .080*
      
       (.225)
       (.159)
      
       .003
       -.032
      
       (.016)
       (-.153)
      
       -.094*
       .044
      
       (-.156)
       (.074)
      
       -.026
       .061
      
       (-.062)
       (.144)
      
       -.061
       .045
      
       (-.071)
       (.052)
      
       .050
       .017
      
       (.043)
       (.019)
      
      
       204**
      
      
       (1.773)
      
      
       -.268**
      
      
       (-.452)
      
      
       -.124*
      
      
       (-.270)
      
      
       -.205**
      
      
       (-.224)
      
      
       .049
      
      
       (.066)

    .234

       .258
       .473
       CESD! (Depression)
       Male (=1)
       Age
       Married (=1)
       Employed (=1)
       Education
       Years-Canada
       Income
       Chronic Strain (CS,)
       Life Events (LE,)
       Mastery (MAO
       Self-Esteem (SE,)
       Ethnic Support (KSS,)
       General Support (GSS,)
       Wave 2 Measures Life Events (LE2) Mastery (MA2) Self-Esteem (SE2) Korean Support (KSS2) General Support (GSS2)
       R2
       a Unstandardized coefficients are shown in parentheses. *p< .05; **p< .01.
      
       200
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       displayed in model 2, both chronic strain and mastery have significant effects on respon­dents' distress scores. The addition of Wl measures of stressors and resource variables accounted for an additional 2.4 percent of the variance in the change of depression over time.
       Model 3, the final step in this analysis, displays the results of the regression equation with all W2 stressors and resources added. The inclusion of the contemporaneous mea­sures of stressors and resources increases the variance explained in W2 CES-D scores to 47.3 percent and reveals a number of interesting findings. First, the number of life events experienced by respondents over the one-year period of study is significantly associated with an increase in CES-D scores. Second, increased levels of both mastery and self-esteem are associated with declines in levels of depressive symptoms. Third, in­creases in social support from Korean family and friends contributes to lower CES-D scores; however, general social support has no such effect. One other finding deserves comment. In model 3, the effect of chronic strain measured at Wl persists even after all W2 variables are entered into the equation.
       This attests to the persistence of chronic strain over time.
       Indirect Influences
       While the previous analysis provides clear evidence of the direct effects of changes in stressors and resources on changes in depressive symptomatology, a central con­cern of the stress process paradigm is to specify the interrelationships among stres­sors, resources, and distress. However, as we have mentioned earlier, there are compet­ing models of the relationships among stressors, resources, and distress that need to be assessed empirically in order to better understand the dynamics of the stress process among Korean immigrants.
       Figure 1 presents the results of a path analysis that specifies both direct and indirect effects of the major constructs under consid­eration. In this path model, all path coeffi­cients were estimated from saturated models similar to that presented in model 3 in Table 1, including controls for the effects of sociodemographic factors. The model in Figure 1 includes all Wl and W2 measures of stressors and resources except GSSX and
      
       FIGURE 1. Direct and Indirect Associations Among Stressors, Psychosocial Coping Resources, and Distress*
      
       LE-
       .335
       KSS
       SE
       MA
       0x08 graphic
    CES-Dr
       .272
       CES-D2
      
       * All path coefficients were estimated for the effects of demographic variables (shown in tab. 1) on each endogenous variable. Neither GSS, nor GSS2 was associated with CESD2, directly or indirectly, and therefore are not shown in the figure. All path coefficients are significant atp < .05.
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       201
      
      
      
       GSS2, both of which had nonsignificant paths Interaction Effects
       to CESD2.
       The results in Figure 1 highlight a number of important processes. First, the stability coefficients for all constructs measured at both points in time are relatively strong. Second, psychological resources at Wl ap­pear to reinforce subsequent levels of both social and psychological resources. Self-esteem enhances subsequent levels of ethnic social support and mastery; mastery has a similar effect on self-esteem and ethnic social support. Third, in addition to its direct effect on distress, ethnic social support also has an indirect effect through stressful life events. Thus, it appears that higher levels of ethnic social support have a stress suppression effect. While life events have a direct effect on CES-D scores, they also exert indirect influence through self-esteem. It should be noted, however, that this effect is positive: exposure to stressful events appears to increase subsequent levels of self-esteem which, in turn, results in a decrease in psychological distress. This pattern corre­sponds to what Ensel and Lin (1991) refer to as a counteractive coping model. Finally, our longitudinal analysis reveals that chronic strain has a direct effect on subsequent depression. This impact appears to be unme-diated by any social or psychological resource variable.
       To further investigate the stress process models, the contributions of various sets of interactions (between stressors and resources) were compared. Following Ensel and Lin's (1991) general analytic strategy, we computed three sets of interaction terms. The first set consisted of eight interactions representing the joint effects of each Wl stressor (LEl5 CSj and each W2 resource (MA2, SE2, KSS2, GSS2). Tests of these interaction effects assess whether stress-buffering effects can be observed. The second set, representing what Ensel and Lin refer to as stress-conditioning effects, involves the four interac­tions between Wl resources (MA1? SEl9 KSS1? GSS^ and life events at W2 (LE2). The third set tests interactions between life events at W2 and each resource variable at W2 (MA2, SE2, KSS2, GSS2) to assess contemporaneous stress-resource interactions.
       The results of these analyses are summa­rized in Table 2. In equation I, the regression model including the sociodemographic vari­ables and all Wl and W2 variables (model 4 in tab. 1) has an R2 of .473. This is the baseline model against which all other interaction models are evaluated. In equation II, the addition of stress-buffering interactions increases the variance explained to .483. This increase of .010 over the baseline model is
      
       TABLE 2. Explained Variance in CES-D Scores at W2: R2 Increments Associated With the Addition of Various Sets of Interaction Terms
      
       0x08 graphic
    Equa-ation
       Predictors
       R2 Increment from Equation I
      
      
      
       0x08 graphic
    I: Main Effects: Control Variables, Wl and W2 Predictors1
       II: Stress-Buffering: Equation I Plus Interactions Between Wl
       Stressors and W2 Resources2
       IIA: IIB:
       Psychological Resources3
       Social Resources4
    III: Stress-Conditioning: Equation I Plus Interactions Between Wl
       Resources and W2 Stressors5
    IIIA: Psychological Resources6
    IIIB: Social Resources7
    IV: Contemporaneous Interactions: Equation I Plus Interactions
       Between W2 Stressors and Resources8
      
       473
      
      
       483
       .010
       ns
       482
       .009
       ns
       476
       .003
       ns
       541
       .078
       <.001
       540
       .077
       <.001
       475
       .002
       ns
       482
       .009
       ns
      
      
      
       0x08 graphic
    1 See Model 4, Table 1.
       2 Eight interaction terms between Wl
       3 Four interaction terms between Wl
       4 Four interaction terms between Wl
       5 Four interaction terms between Wl
       6 Two interaction terms between Wl
       7 Two interaction terms between Wl
       8 Four interaction terms between W2
       stressors (LE,, CS,) and W2 resources (MA2, ES2, KSS2, GSS2) stressors (LE,, CS,) and W2 psychological resources (MA2, ES2) stressors (LE,, CS,) and W2 social resources (KSS2, GSS2) resources (MA,, ES,, KSS,, GSS,) and W2 stressors (LE2) psychological resources (MA,, ES,) and W2 stressors (LE2) social resources (KSS,, GSS,) and W2 stressors (LE2) resources (MA2, ES2, KSS2, GSS2) and W2 stressors (LE2)
      
       202
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       nonsignificant. Not surprisingly, when we decompose these interactions into those in­volving psychological resources (mastery or self-esteem) and those involving social re­sources (general social support or ethnic social support), we see that the increments in R2 associated with each equation are small.
       A different pattern emerges, however, when we test for stress-conditioning effects. The inclusion of the four interactions between each of the Wl resources by LE2 in equation III significantly increases the explained vari­ance by .078. When we decompose this increase, we can see that the major compo­nent of this stress-conditioning effect is due to the moderation of stressful life events by psychological resources, mastery, and self-esteem. More precisely, the significant contri­bution is from mastery (MAt * LE2). In the last equation (equation IV), it can be seen that the contemporaneous interactions of life events (LE2) with resources (MA2, SE2, KSS2, GSS2) do not significantly increase the explained variance in CES-D scores. Thus, neither social nor psychological resources modify the concurrent effects of life events on distress among respondents in this study.
       DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
       The major focus of this paper has been to determine whether a stress process formula­tion is applicable to the study of the immigrant experience. In our view, the results of this investigation demonstrate that this paradigm provides a clear depiction of the ways in which stressors, psychosocial coping resources, and distress among Korean immi­grants are interrelated. In this context, there are several points that deserve some discus­sion.
       Earlier, we noted that none of the sociode-mographic variables is significantly associ­ated with changes in CES-D scores over time. While some might argue that such findings challenge Pearlin's (1989) argument for the importance of understanding how social characteristics influence the experience of distress, we believe that a different interpreta­tion of these results can be made, one which is not inconsistent with Pearlin's position. In cross-sectional analyses, we found significant associations between CES-D scores and age, gender, and the length of residence in Canada. These associations disappeared,
       however, when Wl depression was con­trolled. In other words, age, gender, and length of residence are not related to the change in depression. We believe that this pattern reflects the fact that these social factors make their influence on depression through the Wl measure.
       Our analyses also reveal that psychological resources play a central role in the stress process among Korean immigrants. Self-esteem and mastery not only have direct and indirect effects on immigrants' psychological distress; self-esteem is also influenced by prior stressful experience. Our assessment of interaction effects also reveals that earlier levels of mastery moderate the effect of subsequent stressful life events on psycholog­ical distress.
       While social resources apparently play a less significant part in the process through which Korean immigrants experience dis­tress, ethnic social support nevertheless has a direct effect on depressive symptomatology as well as an indirect effect through subsequent life events. Moreover, an interesting observa­tion is that general social support from the broader community plays almost no role in this process, a pattern that we have discussed at length in a previous paper (cf. Noh et al. 1994). While it may be a truism to state that not all sources of support are equally effective in reducing psychological distress, our results provide convincing evidence that is consistent with Thoits' (1986) hypothesis of sociocul-tural similarity. Thoits emphasizes the impor­tance of empathic understanding of stressful situations by providers of support. She suggests that this is more likely to occur when there is sociocultural or situational similarity between the support provider and receiver.
       It seems implicit in Thoits' argument that the key to effective support is a complemen­tarity between the needs and values of both parties in any social support transaction. Sociocultural similarity increases the affinity between the provider and receiver of support. In our study, it appears that social support from members of their own culture has made substantially greater contributions to Korean immigrants' well-being than has support from individuals from the broader community. These findings suggest that new immigrants may be likely to perceive other immigrants as having common life experiences and to expect that they will be better able to understand the exact nature of new immi-
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       203
      
      
      
       grants' adjustment difficulties. In contrast, members of the larger community (particu­larly those of dominant groups) may be viewed as less aware or sensitive to the particular difficulties faced by immigrants. Accordingly, assistance offered by nonethnic significant others may be perceived as inappropriate or even controlling or demand­ing. When the situational difficulties (stres­sors) are particularly relevant to new immi­grants' adjustment processes (such as facing social discrimination or experiencing a sense of marginality), the probability of perceiving interethnic support as threatening or control­ling is likely to increase.
       These findings, however, do not warrant the conclusion that social support from the broader community has no role to play in the mental health of Korean Canadians. They can be understood only in the context of our sample of immigrants. Because Koreans born in Canada were not included in this study, it seems likely that the potential impact of general social support on Korean-Canadian's mental health is probably attenuated. That is, limiting our study to immigrants probably underestimates the effects of acculturation and its benefits to mental health derived from support from the broader community.
       In our view, these analyses also have implications for testing competing models within the stress process formulation. As we indicated at the outset of this paper, our study affords an opportunity to test different models of the stress process that have been put forth by Ensel and Lin (1991). Our results tend to be more consistent with their concept of deterring models than with coping models. Indeed, elements of our analyses are consis­tent with each of the three deterring models that Lin and Ensel have specified. The observation that chronic strains and social and psychological resources all have significant direct effects on distress is congruent with their independent model. Moreover, our finding that ethnic social support reduces subsequent levels of stressful life events is compatible with their stress-suppressing model. Finally, the observation that Wl mastery interacts with W2 life events is an example of a stress-conditioning model. We find only one instance of a relationship that is consistent with any of the coping models that Ensel and Lin have hypothesized. The experience of eventful stressors results in an increase in subsequent self-esteem which, in
       turn, produces lower levels of distress. In Ensel and Lin's framework, this corresponds to a counteractive model. While some might find such patterns to be counterintuitive, they may be consistent with the observation that some stressful experience can provide oppor­tunities for personal growth and the develop­ment of better coping abilities (Turner and Avison 1992b).
       Thus, with this exception, our results suggest that the relationships among stressors, resources, and distress among Korean immi­grants may best be conceptualized as a deterrent process. In this process, psycholog­ical and social resources directly reduce levels of distress and either inhibit the occurrence of subsequent stressors or reduce the likelihood that they will be distressful in their conse­quences.
       REFERENCES
       Aneshensel, Carol S. and J.D. Stone. 1982. "Stress and Depression: A Test of the Buffering Model of Social Support." Archives of General Psychiatry 39:1392-96.
       Avison, William R. 1995. "Roles and Resources: The Effects of Work and Family Context on Women's Psychosocial Resources and Psycho­logical Distress." Pp. 233-56 in Research in Community and Mental Health, vol. 8, edited by J.R. Greenley. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
       Banchevska, Rachelle. 1981. "Uprooting and Settling: The Transplanted Family." Pp. 107-32 in Strangers in the World, edited by L. Fitinger and D. Schwartz. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
       Beiser, Morton and Merry Wood. 1986. Canadian Task Force on Mental Health Issues Affecting Immigrants and Refugees: Review of the Litera­ture on Migrant Mental Health. Ottawa, Can­ada: Health and Welfare Canada.
       Berk, Bernard and Lucie C. Hirata. 1973. "Mental Illness Among the Chinese: Myth or Reality." Journal of Social Issues 29:149-66.
       Bland, Roger C, Stephen C. Newman, and Helen Orn. 1988. "Period Prevalence of Psychiatric Disorders in Edmonton." Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia (Suppl. 338) 77:33-42.
       Burnam, M. Audrey, Richard L. Hough, Marvin Karno, Javier J. Escobar, and Cynthia A. Telles. 1987. "Acculturation and Lifetime Prevalence of Psychiatric Disorders Among Mexican Americans in Los Angeles." Journal of Health Social Behavior 28:89-102.
       Cochrane, Raymond and Mary Stopes-Roe. 1977. "Psychological and Social Adjustment of Asian Immigrants to Britain: A Community Survey." Social Psychiatry 12:195-206.
      
       204
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       Dohrenwend, Barbara S. and Bruce P. Dohren-wend. 1981. "Life Stress and Illness: Formula­tion of the Issues." Pp. 1-27 in Stressful Life Events and Their Contexts, edited by B.S. Dohrenwend and B.P. Dohrenwend. New York: Prodist.
       Dumas, Jean. 1985. Current Demographic Analy­sis: Report on the Demographic Situation in Canada. Ottawa, Canada: Ministry of Supply and Services Canada.
       Eaton, William W. and Larry G. Kessler. 1981. "Rates of Symptoms of Depression in a National Sample." American Journal of Epidemiology 96:319-25.
       Ensel, Walter M. and Nan Lin. 1991. "The Life Stress Paradigm and Psychological Distress." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 32:321-42.
       Fitinger, Leo and David Schwartz. 1981. Strang­ers in the World. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
       Frerichs, Ralph R., Carol S. Aneshensel, and Virginia A. Clark. 1981. "Prevalence of De­pression in Los Angeles County." American Journal of Epidemiology 86:474-95.
       Gary, Lawrence E., Diane Robinson Brown, Norweeta G. Milburn, Feroz Ahmed, and Jacqueline Booth. 1989. Depression in Black American Adults: Findings from the Norfolk Area Health Study. Washington, DC: Institute of Urban Affairs.
       Gordon, Milton M. 1962. Assimilation in Ameri­can Life. New York: Oxford University Press.
       Gore, Susan. 1981. "Stress Buffering Functions of Social Supports: An Appraisal and Clarification of Research Models." Pp. 202-22 in Stressful Life Events and Their Contexts, edited by В .S. Dohren­wend and B.P. Dohrenwend. New York: Prodist.
       Granovetter, Mark S. 1973. "The Strength of Weak Ties." American Journal of Sociology 78:1360-80.
       Hirsch, Barton J. 1980. "Psychological Dimen­sions of Social Networks: A Multimethod Analysis." American Journal of Community Psychology 7:263-77.
       House, James. 1981. Work Stress and Social Support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
       House, James, Karl R. Landis, and Debra Umberson. 1988. "Structures and Processes of Social Support." Annual Review of Sociology 14:293-318.
       Hull, Diana. 1979. "Migration, Adaptation, and Illness: A Review." Social Science and Medi­cine 13:25-36.
       Hurh, Won M. and Kwang C. Kim. 1984. Korean Immigrants in America: A Structural Analysis of Ethnic Confinement and Adhesive Adaptation. Cranbury, NJ: Fairleigh Dickinson University Press.
       1988. Uprooting and Adjustment: A
       Sociological Study of Korean Immigrants' Men-
       tal Health. Final Report to National Institute of Mental Health. Macomb, IL: Department of Sociology and Anthropology. Western Illinois University.
       _. 1990. "Adaptation Stages and Mental
       Health of Korean Male Immigrants in the United States." International Migration Review 24: 456-79.
       Jew, Charles C. and Stuart A. Brody. 1967. "Mental Illness Among the Chinese: Hospital-ization Rates Over the Past Century." Compar­ative Psychiatry 8:129-34.
       Kaplan, Howard B. 1975. Self-Attitudes and Deviant Behavior. Pacific Palisades, CA: Good-year.
       Kintano, Harry H. 1969. "Japanese-American Mental Illness." Pp. 256-84 in Changing Perspectives in Mental Illness, edited by S.C. Plog and R.B. Edgerton. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
       Koranyi, Erwin K. 1981. "Decline and Stress: Immigrants' Adaptation in an Aged Popula­tion." Pp. 220-32 in Strangers in the World, edited by L. Fitinger and D. Schwartz. Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
       Kuo, Wen H. 1976. "Theories of Migration and Mental Health: An Empirical Testing on Chinese Americans." Social Science and Medicine 10: 297-306.
       1984. "Prevalence of Depression Among
       Asian-Americans." Journal of Nervous Mental Disease 172:449-57.
       Kuo, Wen H. and Yung-mei Tsai. 1986. "Social Networking, Hardiness, and Immigrants' Mental Health." Journal of Health Social Behavior 27:133-49.
       Lefcourt, Herbert M. 1981. "Locus of Control and Stressful Life Events." Pp. 157-66 in Stressful Life Events and Their Contexts, edited by B.S. Dohrenwend and B.P. Dohrenwend. New York: Wiley.
       Lin, Ken-Ming, Thomas S. Inui, Arthur M. Kleinman, and William M. Womack. 1982. "Sociocultural Determinants of the Help-Seeking Behavior of Patients with Mental Illness." Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 170:78-85.
       Lin, Nan. 1989. "Measuring Depressive Symp­tomatology in China." Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 177:121-31.
       1992. "Social Resources Theory." Pp.
       1936-42 in Encyclopedia of Sociology, edited by E.F. Borgatta and M.L. Borgatta. New York: Macmillan Publishing.
       Lin, Nan and Walter M. Ensel. 1989. "Life Stress and Health: Stressors and Resources." American Sociological Review 54:382-99.
       Lin, Nan, Alfred Dean, and Walter M. Ensel. 1986. Social Support, Life Events, and Depres­sion. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
      
       Korean Immigrants and the Stress Process
       205
      
      
      
       Lin, Nan, Ronald Simeone, Walter M. Ensel, and Wen H. Kuo. 1979. "Social Support, Stressful Life Events, and Illness: A Model and an Empirical Test." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 20:108-19.
       Moritsugu, John and Stanley Sue. 1983. "Minority Status as a Stressor." Pp. 162-74 in Preventive Psychology: Theory, Research, and Practice, edited by R.D. Felner, L.A. Jason, J.N. Moritsugu, and S.S. Farber. New York: Perga-mon Press.
       Murphy, Henry Brian Meggett. 1973. "Migration and the Major Mental Disorders: A Reap­praisal." Pp. 204-20 in Uprooting and After, edited by C. Zwingmann and M. Pfister-Ammende. New York: Springer-Verlag.
       Murphy, Jane M., Arthur M. Sobol, and Raymond K. Neff. 1984. "Stability of Prevalence: Depres­sion and Anxiety Disorders." Archives of General Psychiatry 41:990-97.
       Noh, Samuel and R. Jay Turner. 1987. "Living With Psychiatric Patients: Implications for the Mental Health of Family Members." Social Science and Medicine 25:263-71.
       Noh, Samuel, William R. Avison, and Violet Kaspar. 1992. "Depressive Symptoms Among Korean Immigrants: Assessment of a Translation of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale." Psychological Assessment 4:84-91.
       Noh, Samuel, Zheng Wu, and William R Avison. 1994. "Social Support and Quality of Life: Sociocultural Similarity and the Efficacy of Social Support." Pp. 115-37 in Advances in Medical Sociology, vol. v: Quality of Life in Health Care, edited by G. Albrecht and R. Fitzpatrick. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
       Noh, Samuel, Mark Speechley, Violet Kaspar, and Zheng Wu. 1992. "Depression in Korean Immigrants in Canada: I. Method of the Study and Prevalence." Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 180:573-77.
       Noh, Samuel, Zheng Wu, Mark Speechley, and Violet Kaspar. 1992. "Depression in Korean Immigrants in Canada: II. Effects of Gender, Work, and Marriage." Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 180:578-82.
       Paykel, Eugene S. 1978. "Contribution of Life Events to Causation and Psychiatric Illness." Psychological Medicine 8:245-51.
       Pearlin, Leonard I. 1989. "The Sociological Study of Stress." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 30:241-56
       Pearlin, Leonard I. and Carmi Schooler. 1978. "The Structure of Coping." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 19:2-21.
       Pearlin, Leonard I., Morton A. Lieberman, Elizabeth G. Menaghan, and Joseph T. Mullan. 1981. "The Stress Process." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 22:337-56.
       Portes, Alejandro. 1983. "Models of Structural
       Incorporation." Pp. 279-97 in Global Trends in Migration, edited by M. Kritz, C. Keeley, and S. Tomasi. New York: Center for Migration Studies.
       Portes, Alejandro and Ruben G. Rumbaut. 1990. Immigrant America: A Portrait. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
       Quisumbing, Maria Socorro. 1982. Life Events, Social Support, and Personality: Their Impact on Filipino Psychological Adjustment. Doctoral Dissertation, Department of Psychology, Uni­versity of Chicago.
       Regier, Darrel A., Jerome K. Myers, and Morton Kramer. 1984. "The NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program: Historical Context, Major Objectives, and Study Population Charac­teristics." Archives of General Psychiatry 41: 934-41.
       Robins, Lee N. and Darrel A. Regier, 1991. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York: Free Press.
       Rosenberg, Morris. 1979. Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books.
       Rotter, Julian B. 1966. "Generalized Expectancies for Internal vs. External Control of Reinforce­ment." Psychological Monographs 80:1-28.
       Rumbaut, Ruben G. 1991. "The Agony of Exile: A Study of the Migration and Adaptation of Indochinese Refugee Adults and Children." Pp. 53--91 in Refugee Children: Theory, Research, and Services, edited by F.L. Ahearn and J.L. Athey. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Univer­sity Press.
       Sarason, Irwin G., Gregory R. Pierce, and Barbara R. Sarason. 1994. "General and Specific Perceptions of Social Support." Pp. 151-77 in Stress and Mental Health: Contemporary Issues and Prospects for the Future, edited by W.R. Avison and I.H. Gotlib. New York: Plenum Press.
       Seligman, Martin E.P. 1975. Helplessness: On Development, Depression, and Death. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
       Shin, Y. Edward. 1991. "Statistics on Korean Canadians in Canada: Second Report of Com­munity Survey on Existing and Future Potential of Korean Canadian Population Resources, Phase 1." Presented at the Community Confer­ence on Family, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
       Sue, Stanley. 1977. "Psychological Theory and Implications for Asian Americans." Personnel and Guidance Journal 55:381-89.
       Sue, Stanley and Herman McKinney. 1975. "Asian Americans in the Community Mental Health Care System." American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 45:111-18.
       Sue, Stanley and J. Morishima. 1982. The Mental Health of Asian Americans. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
       Sue, Stanley and Nolan Zane. 1980. "Learned Helplessness Theory and Community Psychol-
      
       206
       JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
      
      
      
       ogy." Pp. 121-43 in Community Psychology: Theoretical and Empirical Approaches, edited by M. Gibbs, J. Lachenmeyer, and J. Sigal. New York: Gardner.
       Thoits, Peggy A. 1982. "Conceptual, Methodo­logical, and Theoretical Problems in Studying Social Support as a Buffer Against Life Stress." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 23:145-59.
       1986. "Social Support as Coping Assis­
    tance." Journal of Consulting and Clinical
    Psychology 54:416-23.
       Turner, R. Jay. 1981. "Experienced Social Sup­port as a Contingency in Emotional Weil-Being." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 22:324-36.
       Turner, R. Jay and William R. Avison. 1992a. "Sources of Attenuation in the Stress-Distress Relationship: An Evaluation of Modest Innova­tions in the Application of Event Checklists." Pp. 269-94 in Research in Community and Mental Health, vol. 7, edited by J.R. Greenley and P. Leaf. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
       1992b. "Innovations in the Measurement
       of Life Stress: Crisis Theory and the Signifi­cance of Event Resolution." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 33:36-50.
       Turner, R. Jay and Samuel Noh. 1983. "Class and Psychological Vulnerabilities Among Women: The Significance of Social Support and Personal Control." Journal of Health and Social Behav­ior 24:2-15.
       1988. "Physical Disability and Depres-
       sion: A Longitudinal Analysis." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 29:23-27.
       Turner, R. Jay and Patricia Roszell. 1994. "Psychosocial Resources and the Stress Pro­cess." Pp. 179-212 in Stress and Mental Health: Contemporary Issues and Prospects for the Future, edited by W.R. Avison and I.H. Gotlib. New York: Plenum Press.
       Turner, R. Jay, B. Gail Frankel, and Deborah Levin. 1983. "Social Support: Conceptualiza­tion, Measurement, and Implications for Mental Health." Pp. 67-111 in Research in Community and Mental Health, vol. 3, edited by J. Greenley. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
       Vega, William A. and Ruben G. Rumbaut. 1991. "Ethnic Minorities and Mental Health." Annual Review of Sociology 17:351-83.
       Weiss, Robert S. 1974. "The Provisions of Social Relationships." Pp. 17-26 in Doing Unto Others, edited by Z. Rubin. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
       Wellman, Barry. 1981. "Applying Network Anal­ysis to the Study of Support." Pp. 171-200 in Social Networks and Social Support, edited by B. Gottlieb. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
       Wheaton, Blair. 1985. "Models for the Stress-Buffering Functions of Coping Resources." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 26:352-64.
       Wilcox, Brian L. 1981. "Social Support, Life Stress, and Psychological Adjustment: A Test of the Buffering Hypothesis." American Journal of Community Psychology 9:371-86.
      
       Samuel Noh is associate professor in the Departments of Occupational Therapy and Epidemiology and Biostatistics at the University of Western Ontario. His current research interests include the investigation of psychosocial processes of stress. Most recently, his interests have focused on cross-cultural aspects of the stress process.
       William R. Avison is professor of sociology and psychiatry and Director of the Centre for Health and Weil-Being at the University of Western Ontario. He currently holds a Senior Research Fellowship from the Ontario Mental Health Foundation. With Ian H. Gotlib, he is the editor of Stress and Mental Health: Contemporary Issues and Prospects for the Future (Plenum Press). In addition to his research with Samuel Noh on immigrants' mental health, he is pursuing a program of research on socioeconomic disadvantage and family mental health. In this context, he is conducting a longitudinal study of unemployment and distress and disorder in families and a study of the effectiveness of a brief home-based intervention for teen mothers and their infants.
  • Комментарии: 2, последний от 08/08/2014.
  • © Copyright Samuel Noh, William R. Avison (kazgugnk@yahoo.com)
  • Обновлено: 30/01/2011. 150k. Статистика.
  • Статья: Канада
  •  Ваша оценка:

    Связаться с программистом сайта
    "Заграница"
    Путевые заметки
    Это наша кнопка